Visual Acuity




Visual acuity is a measure of the keenness of sight. The Egyptians used the ability to distinguish double stars as a measure of visual acuity more than 5000 years ago. Over the centuries visual acuity has been studied, measured and analyzed because it represents a fundamental limit in our ability to see. Consequently, visual acuity has been used as a criterion for military service and various other occupations, driving, and for receiving social security benefits.


Visual acuity is limited primarily by the optics of the eye and by the anatomy and physiology of the visual system. As such, visual acuity is perhaps the key clinical measure of the integrity of the optical and physiological state of the eye and visual pathways ( Box 33.1 ).



Box 33.1

Visual Acuity


Visual acuity is a measure of the keenness of sight. Visual acuity is limited primarily by the optics of the eye and by the anatomy and physiology of this visual system. As such, visual acuity is perhaps the key clinical measure of the integrity of the optical and physiological state of the eye and visual pathways.



Defining and specifying visual acuity


How do we define the keenness of sight? Visual acuity is used to specify a spatial limit, i.e. a threshold in the spatial dimension. Over the centuries there have been a large number of different ideas about how to define, measure and specify visual acuity, and these can be distilled down to four widely accepted criteria:




  • Minimum visible acuity – detection of a feature



  • Minimum resolvable acuity – resolution of two features



  • Minimum recognizable acuity – identification of a feature



  • Minimum discriminable acuity – discrimination of a change in a feature (e.g. a change in size, position or orientation).



These different criteria actually represent different limits and may be determined by different aspects of the visual pathway ( Table 33.1 ).



Table 33.1

Summary of the different forms of acuity and their limits
























Type of acuity Measured Acuity (degrees)
Minimum visible Detection of a feature 0.00014
Minimum resolvable Resolution of two features 0.017
Minimum recognizable Identification of a feature 0.017
Minimum discriminable Discrimination of a change in a feature 0.00024


Minimum visible acuity





  • Minimum visible acuity refers to the smallest object that one can detect.



As early as the 17th century, de Valdez measured the distance at which a row of mustard seeds could no longer be counted, and early astronomers like Robert Hooke were interested in the size of stars that could be detected and their relation to retinal anatomy. In this context, the minimum visible acuity refers to the smallest target that can be detected. Under ideal conditions, humans can detect a long, dark wire (like the cables of the Golden Gate bridge) against a very bright background (like the sky on a bright sunny day) when they subtend an angle of just 0.5 arc seconds (≈ 0.00014 degrees). It is widely accepted that the minimum visible acuity is so small because the optics of the eye (described below) spread the image of the thin line, so that on the retina it is much wider, and the fuzzy retinal image of the wire casts a shadow which reduces the light on a row of cones to a level which is just detectably less than the light on the row of cones on either side. In other words, although we specify the minimum visible acuity in terms of the angular size of the target at the retina, it is actually limited by our ability to discriminate the intensity of the target relative to its background.


Increasing the target size, up to a point, is equivalent to increasing its relative intensity. Hecht & Mintz measured the visual resolution of a black line against a background over a large range of background brightness. They found that the minimum visible acuity varied from about 10 minutes at the lowest background levels, to about 0.5 arc seconds at the highest. The limiting factor in the case of minimum visible acuity is the observers’ sensitivity to small variations in the stimulus intensity (ΔI/I), i.e. their contrast sensitivity (discussed below). Indeed, Hecht & Mintz state that the retinal image produced by the 0.5 arc second line represents a “fine fuzz of a shadow extending over several cones”, and they calculated that at the highest intensity levels tested, the foveal cones occupying the center of the shadow suffers a drop in intensity (relative to the neighboring cones) of ≈ 1 percent – just at the limit of intensity discrimination.


Although the minimum visible acuity represents one limit to spatial vision, it is in fact a limit in the ability to discern small changes in contrast, rather than a spatial limit per se, and minimum visible acuity is not used clinically.


Minimum resolvable acuity





  • Minimum resolvable acuity refers to the smallest angular separation between neighboring objects that one can resolve.



More than 5000 years ago the Egyptians assessed visual acuity by the ability of an observer to resolve double stars. There is currently still debate about how best to define and measure resolution. However, today, the minimum resolvable acuity is much more likely to be assessed by determining the finest black and white stripes that can be resolved. Under ideal conditions (e.g. high contrast and luminance), humans with very good vision can resolve black and white stripes when one cycle subtends an angle of approximately 1 minute of arc (0.017 degrees). This minimum resolvable acuity represents one of the fundamental limits of spatial vision: it is the finest high-contrast detail that can be resolved. In foveal vision the limit is determined primarily by the spacing of photoreceptors in the retina. The visual system “samples” the stripes discretely, through the array of receptors at the back of the retina ( Fig. 33.1 ). If the receptors are spaced such that the whitest and blackest parts of the grating fall on separate cones ( Fig. 33.1B ), we should be able to make out the grating. But if the entire cycle falls on a single cone ( Fig. 33.1C ), we will see nothing but a gray field (or we may experience a phenomenon called aliasing , in which we misperceive the width or orientation of the stripes). Cones in the fovea have a center-to-center separation of about 0.5 minutes of arc (0.008 degrees), which fits nicely with the observed acuity limit of 1 minute of arc (0.017 degrees – since we need two cones per cycle to be able to perceive it accurately), and each foveal cone has a “private” line to a ganglion cell. Rods and cones in the retinal periphery are less tightly packed together, and many receptors converge on each ganglion cell. As a result, visual acuity is much poorer in the periphery than in the fovea (more about that later).




Figure 33.1


Spatial sampling of a sinusoid whose spatial frequency is ( A ) lower than the sampling limit set by the spacing of the cones, ( B ) equal to the sampling limit set by the spacing of the cones, ( C ) higher than the sampling limit set by the spacing of the cones.


Minimum recognizable acuity





  • Minimum recognizable acuity refers to the angular size of the smallest feature that one can recognize or identify.



Although this method has been used since the 17th century, the approach still used by eye doctors today was introduced more than a century ago by Herman Snellen and his colleagues. Snellen constructed a set of block letters for which the letter as a whole was five times as large as the strokes that formed the letter ( Fig. 33.2 ). The distance of the patient was varied until they could no longer accurately read the letters. In later adaptations of the Snellen test, the viewer was positioned at a constant distance (typically 20 feet [6 meters]), and the size of the letters, rather than the position of the viewer, was altered and visual acuity was defined as:




Figure 33.2


Snellen acuity chart.


The distance at which the patient can just identify the letters / The distance at which a person with “normal” vision can just identify the letters


Thus, “normal” vision came to be defined as 20/20 (6/6 in metric units). To relate this back to visual angle, a 20/20 letter is designed to subtend an angle of 5 arc minutes (0.083 degrees) at the eye, and each stroke of a 20/20 letter subtends an angle of 0.017 degrees (our old familiar 1 arc minute). Thus, if you can read a 20/20 letter, you can discern detail that subtends 1 minute of arc. If you have to be at 20 feet to read a letter that someone with normal vision can read at 40 feet, you have 20/40 vision (worse than normal). Although 20/20 is often considered the gold standard, most healthy young adults have an acuity level closer to 20/15. Illiterate E and Landolt C charts are based on the same principles as the Snellen chart.


Although Snellen’s notation for visual acuity is commonly used, there are other schemes for specifying acuity ( Table 33.2 ). For example, the Minimum Angle of Resolution (MAR) is the angular size of detail of the visual acuity letter in minutes of arc – it is the Snellen denominator (the number below the line, e.g. 40) over the Snellen numerator (the number above the line, e.g. 20). Thus, for example a Snellen acuity of 20/20 is equivalent to an MAR of 1′; 20/40 to an MAR of 2′ and 20/100, an MAR of 5′. Another method for specifying acuity is the Snellen fraction (the Snellen numerator divided by the denominator or 1/MAR). Acuity is sometimes specified as LogMAR (the logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution) and sometimes as Log Visual Acuity (the logarithm of the Snellen fraction). Snell & Sterling developed a metric for quantifying visual loss due to injury or disease. The Snell–Sterling efficiency scale sets 20/20 (MAR = 1′) as 100% efficiency, and reduces the efficiency by a fixed percentage (≈ 84%) for every 1 minute loss of acuity.



Table 33.2

Different acuity notations
































Snellen (Imperial) Notation (Metric) MAR logMAR Decimal SF (c/deg)
20/200 6/60 10 1 0.1 3
20/20 6/6 1 0.0 1.00 30
20/10 6/3 0.5 −0.3 2 60


There are many stimulus and subject variables that influence the minimum recognizable visual acuity (discussed below).


Minimum discriminable acuity





  • Minimum discriminable acuity refers to the angular size of the smallest change in a feature (e.g. a change in size, position or orientation) that one can discriminate.



Perhaps the most studied example of minimum discriminable acuity is our ability to discern a difference in the relative positions of two features. Our visual systems are very good at telling where things are. Consider two abutting horizontal lines, one slightly higher than the other ( Fig 33.3A ). It is very easy to discern that, for example the right line is higher than the left (i.e. discriminate the relative positions of the two lines) even from a long way away. This is an example of a class of visual tasks that have been given the label “ hyperacuity ” by Gerald Westheimer. These tasks all have in common that they involve judging the relative position of objects, and Westheimer coined the term hyperacuity, because, under ideal conditions, humans can make these judgments with a precision that is finer than the size or spacing of foveal cones.




Figure 33.3


Two “hyperacuity” configurations: Vernier acuity ( A ) and bisection acuity ( B ). In both cases, the subject’s task is to judge the relative position of the features.


The smallest misalignment that we can reliably discern is known as Vernier acuity – named after the Frenchman, Pierre Vernier, whose scale, developed in the 17th century, was widely used to aid ship’s navigators. The success of the Vernier scale was based on the fact that humans are very adept at judging whether nearby lines are lined up or not. Thus, Vernier alignment is still widely used in precision machines, and even in the dial switches in modern ovens. Under ideal conditions, Vernier acuity may be just three arc seconds (≈ 0.0008 degrees)! This performance is even more remarkable when you consider that it is about ten times smaller than even the smallest foveal cones. Note that the optics of the eye spread the image of a thin line over a number of retinal cones, and that the eyes are in constant motion, and this performance appears even more remarkable.


Vernier acuity is not the most remarkable form of hyperacuity. Guinness World Records 2005 describes the “highest hyperacuity” as follows: In April 1984, Dr. Dennis M. Levi (yes – that’s yours truly) “… repeatedly identified the relative position of a thin, bright green line within 0.8 seconds of arc (0.00024 deg). This is equivalent to a displacement of some 0.25 inches (6 mm) at a distance of 1 mile (1.6 km).” This “remarkable” position acuity was accomplished with a bisection task ( Fig. 33.3B ), but can actually be understood based on a straightforward model of human spatial vision.


As remarkable as the other hyperacuities (sometimes also called “position” acuities) might seem, they do not defy the laws of physics. Geisler calculated that if you placed a machine (known as an ideal discriminator) at the retina, and this machine knew precisely the pattern of photons absorbed by the retinal photoreceptors when the stripes were aligned, and the pattern of photons absorbed when they were misaligned, that this machine could actually perform an order of magnitude better than even the best humans. So, the information about the Vernier offset is present in the pattern of photons absorbed by the photoreceptors; however, humans must be able to interpret the information despite the constant motion of the eyes. Thus hyperacuity must ultimately be limited by neurons in the visual cortex that are able to interpolate positional information with high resolution.


There is a good deal of evidence that different mechanisms limit position judgments for closely spaced (or abutting) targets, and for widely separated targets. In the nearby case, both contrast and contrast polarity are important. For example, in a two-line Vernier target Vernier acuity is better when the lines are both either bright or dark than if one line is bright and the other dark. For longer-range position judgments where the targets are well separated, neither contrast nor contrast polarity nor the local stimulus details matter very much. For long-range position judgments the visual system must localize each of the features, and then compare the position labels of separate cortical mechanisms. The idea that cortical receptive fields have position labels (in addition to labels for other stimulus dimensions) is consistent with the topographical mapping of visual space in the brain (i.e. each point in space is systematically mapped on to the visual cortex). More than a century ago, Hermann Lotze wrote:




“So long as the opinion is maintained that the space relations of impressions pass as such into the soul, it must of course, in the interest of the soul, be further held that each impression is conveyed to it by a distinct fibre, and that the fibres reach the seat of the soul with their relative situation wholly undisturbed”.



Although we now know much more about the anatomy and physiology of the visual system than was known in the 1880s, Lotze clearly recognized that there must be a topographical representation of the world in the visual nervous system (if not the soul!), and that each “fibre” must carry a label about the position of the “impression” that it carried. For this reason, position labels were called “Lotze’s local signs”. Lotze also concluded that local signs played an important role in directing eye movements toward stimuli in the peripheral field of vision. So how accurate is “local sign” information? It turns out that humans can localize the position of a single peripheral feature to within about 1–2 percent of the eccentricity of the target, a little more precisely than we can make saccadic eye movements to peripheral targets.




Limiting factors in visual acuity


In this section we explore the optical, anatomical and physiological factors that limit visual acuity. In the fovea, optics anatomy and neural mechanisms conspire to limit our visual acuity.


Optical quality of the eye


The optics of the eye spread the retinal image. Consider the retinal image of a distant star. Because stars are really far away, they are considered to be a “point source”, i.e. if they were in perfect focus and the eye’s optics were perfect, they would subtend an infinitely small angle at the eye. Point sources are useful if you want to learn about the quality of the eye’s optics. As it turns out, the eye’s optics are far from perfect; in fact your camera might have better optical quality. The eye’s optics spread the image, so that a point in space forms a distribution (like a Gaussian) on the retina, as illustrated in Figure 33.4A . This distribution, like the office football pool, is known as the point spread function (if the source is a line it’s called the line spread function). Figure 33.4B–D shows the retinal light distribution for a pair of nearby stars. As the stars become closer and closer together, their images overlap to a greater and greater degree, so that when they are very close, they look like a single distribution. When the separation between the stars on the retina is less than half the spread of each image, they will appear as a single star. This is known as the Rayleigh limit ( Fig. 33.4D ). The Rayleigh limit is determined mainly by the wavelength of the light and the size of the pupil (we’ll soon discuss why). As noted earlier, the ability to distinguish double stars was one of the earliest measures of visual acuity, used by the Egyptians more than 5000 years ago.




Figure 33.4


( A ) Retinal image of a single point. Although the point is infinitesimally small, its image is spread on the retina. ( B–D ) Retinal image of a pair of nearby points, separated by distances equal to two ( B ), one ( C ) and half ( D ) the spread of each image. The latter case is known as the Rayleigh limit.


Another way to determine the quality of an optical system is to measure its modulation transfer function . This is typically done by passing test patterns of sinusoidal gratings ( Fig. 33.5 ) of known contrast through the optical system, and measuring the contrast in the image. Sinewaves are characterized by their spatial frequency (i.e. the number of stripes in a given distance, usually specified in cycles per degree), their contrast (i.e. the difference in the luminance of the peaks and troughs divided by the sum of the luminance of the peaks and troughs), and their phase (the position of the peaks). Sinewaves are especially useful because even after they are degraded by an optical system, they maintain their characteristic shape (i.e. once a sinewave always a sinewave) they just become smaller in amplitude (and they may shift in phase). The ratio of image contrast to object contrast is a measure of image quality. By measuring the ratio of image contrast to object contrast for a range of object spatial frequencies, we can measure the modulation transfer of any optical system. The red line in Figure 33.6A shows the average modulation transfer functions of a large group of observers with 3-mm pupils (about the average size of the pupil in a well-lighted room). As the object spatial frequency increases the modulation transfer declines, falling to near zero at a spatial frequency of around 80 cycles per degree. The spatial frequency at which the modulation transfer function falls to zero is called the cutoff spatial frequency . At this spatial frequency the optics do not transmit the sinusoidal variations in the luminance of the object. The theoretical cutoff spatial frequency (in cycles/degree), like the Rayleigh limit, is determined mainly by the wavelength of the light (λ), and the diameter of the pupil (d):


<SPAN role=presentation tabIndex=0 id=MathJax-Element-1-Frame class=MathJax style="POSITION: relative" data-mathml='CutoffSF(c/deg)=π/180*d/λ’>CutoffSF(c/deg)=π/180*d/λCutoffSF(c/deg)=π/180*d/λ
Cutoff SF ( c / deg ) = π / 180 * d / λ
where d and λ are both specified in millimeters (mm). For red light (630 nm or 0.000630 mm as used by Liang & Williams ) and a 3-mm pupil (about as small as they get) the predicted cutoff spatial frequency is 83 c/deg.


Figure 33.5


The modulation transfer function.



Figure 33.6


The modulation transfer function of the human eye. ( A ) The red line is the mean MTF of 12 eyes with a 3-mm pupil with refractive error corrected. The blue line is the MTF of an aberration-free eye (i.e. limited by diffraction only). The green line shows schematically the effect of uncorrected refractive error.

(Adapted from Liang J. & Williams, D.R. (1997). Aberrations and retinal image quality of the normal human eye. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis., 14, 2873–83. )


What limits optical image formation of the eye?


As noted above, the optics of the human eye are imperfect. Why do the eye’s optics not match up to a fine camera? One limitation is diffraction produced by the pupil of the eye (or the lens aperture of a camera). For example, when light passes through the aperture, instead of staying in a straight line, the light from each ray will be scattered into different directions. This scatter is known as diffraction, and its consequence is to spread or defocus the image and to reduce the transfer of high spatial frequencies in the modulation transfer function. The blue lines in Figure 33.6 show the diffraction limit (i.e. the modulation transfer of an aberration free optical system). As noted above, the cutoff spatial frequency in the modulation transfer function depends on the pupil diameter. The red line in Figure 33.6B shows the modulation transfer function of a large group of human eyes for a large (7-mm) pupil size. Note that for the large pupil diameter the modulation transfer function is much poorer than predicted by the theoretical limit set by diffraction.


In both panels of Figure 33.6 , the eye’s optics are always worse than the theoretical limit, especially when the pupil is large. One important reason is that the eye exhibits aberrations . One form of aberrations in the eye is known as spherical aberrations, in which light rays passing through different parts of the eye’s optics are focussed at slightly different points in the image plane. Larger pupils will result in more aberrations because more peripheral rays of light will enter the eye. There are also chromatic aberrations. If the light source (like starlight) is a mixture of light of different wavelengths, then not all of the wavelengths will be in focus on the retina (at any given time), so the eye will have different point spreads for different wavelengths.


Refractive error and defocus results in a marked loss of image quality


The refractive error of the eye is determined by the refractive power of the optical components of the eye (i.e. the power of the cornea and lens) and the length of the eyeball. To focus a distant point source on the retina, the refractive power of the optical components of the eye must be perfectly matched to the length of the eyeball. This perfect match is known as emmetropia. If there is an uncorrected refractive error or the optics of the eye are defocused, the image of a distant object on the retina will be spread out (i.e. the point spread function will widen) and the modulation transfer function will be low (green line in Fig. 33.6A ).


For myopes, the power of the optical components is too strong for the length of the eyeball, causing the light rays to converge too much to focus on the retina. Myopia cannot be remedied through accommodation because accommodation will increase the convergence of the light. The amount of image spread depends on the amount of defocus, and the pupil size. Reducing pupil size increases the depth of focus, so that defocus has less effect with small than with large pupils. As a rule of thumb, 1 diopter of uncorrected simple myopia will result in a decrease of Snellen acuity, on average, to ≈ 20/60.


Myopia can be corrected with negative (minus) lenses, which diverge the rays. On the other hand, hyperopia (far-sightedness) occurs when the power of the optical components is too weak for the length of the eyeball, causing the light rays to not converge enough to focus on the retina (the image plane lies behind the retina in an unaccommodated hyperopic eye). If the hyperopia is not too severe, a young hyperope can compensate by accommodating, and thereby increasing the power of the eye.


The most powerful refracting surface in the eye is the cornea, which contributes about two-thirds of the eye’s refracting power. When the cornea is not spherical the result is astigmatism. With astigmatism, a point source would not have a single point focus on the retina (i.e. the pointspread function would be asymmetric), and lines of different orientations would be focussed in different planes. Although there are other causes for astigmatism, it is usually caused by asymmetry of the front surface of the cornea. Cylindrical lenses that have two focal points can correct astigmatism (e.g. they have different power in the horizontal and vertical meridians).


Photoreceptor size and spacing; aperture size; the “Nyquist” limit; aliasing


Retinal anatomy plays a very important role in setting the limits for foveal resolution acuity. Foveal cones are arranged in a regular arrangement (sometimes called a triangular array) and are densely packed (see Fig. 33.7 ). This dense packing is critical to fine resolution. The reason is simple. The visual world is represented by continuous variations in light intensity. However, our visual system “samples” the world discretely, i.e. by looking at the light distribution through many small individual photoreceptors. The more closely packed the photoreceptors, the better the light distribution can be represented to the visual nervous system. As illustrated in Figure 33.1 , there is a sampling limit. In order to properly represent the peaks and troughs in the intensity profile of a sinewave, there must be at least two cones for each cycle of the grating. This is known as the Nyquist limit and is represented in Figure 33.1B . More formally, the Nyquist limit occurs when the sinewave has a spatial period (i.e. the peak to peak distance) that is two times the cone spacing. When the spatial period is smaller than twice the cone spacing (i.e. when the sinewave spatial frequency is higher than the Nyquist frequency) the phenomenon known as “aliasing” may occur, i.e. rather than the cones signaling a perfect replica of the sinewave on the retina, the signal is distorted – it appears to be a sinewave with a lower spatial frequency than the original ( Fig. 33.8 ). Since cones in the fovea have a center to center separation of around 0.5 arc minutes, the cone sampling (or Nyquist) limit is a grating spatial period of about 1 minute (i.e. a grating spatial frequency of 60 cycles per degree. Note that because the cones have triangular packing, geometry dictates that the Nyquist frequency will actually be about 15 percent higher or roughly 69 c/deg). This represents a fundamental limitation set by the spacing of the foveal cones, and it is not far off the limitations set by the eye’s optics. Thus, in the fovea, the cone spacing is nicely matched to the eye’s optics. As we’ll see later in this chapter, in peripheral vision cone spacing changes dramatically, while the optics change only a little.


Jan 23, 2019 | Posted by in OPHTHALMOLOGY | Comments Off on Visual Acuity

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access