Skip to main content
Log in

Modern Hebrew prefixoids: description and morpho-syntactic analysis

  • Published:
Morphology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In Modern Hebrew, prefixoids such as tat ‘sub’, χad ‘mono’, etc. appear on nouns and suffixed adjectives. The status of the prefixoid is not clear: in some respects, it behaves like the morphological head, but in others it behaves like the dependent. Focusing on tat ‘sub’, this paper provides both the first thorough description of such an item and a morphological analysis. It is claimed that the prefixoid is always a nominal modifier. In some configurations, the modified nominal structure is null. These claims are shown to account for the selectional restrictions of the prefixoid, as well as the agreement and inflection patterns of the different items with tat.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Kahana (1998) is the only exception I am aware of. This sociolinguistically-oriented dissertation provides a list of prefixoids in Modern Hebrew and traces their gradual introduction into Modern Hebrew.

  2. There has been much syntactic work on the Hebrew Construct State: Borer (1999, 2008, 2013), Danon (2008), Ritter (1988), Shlonsky (2004), Siloni (2001). For its phonological aspects see Faust (2014).

  3. It is no coincidence that these pre-nominal and pre-adjectival particles all have analogues in English. According to the Academy of the Hebrew Language, their introduction into Modern Hebrew was modeled on European languages (Gadish, 2016). In addition to the etymologically Semitic prefixoids in the text, there are also borrowed prefixoids, such as pʁo/anti-, psewdo-, pʁoto- etc.; these cannot carry inflectional markers.

  4. Henceforth, stress is unmarked when final, and marked with an acute accent when penultimate.

  5. Note that the inflectional pattern in (7e–h) is also different from that of Adj+N. In contrast to (7h), the plural form of e.g. ktan emuna ‘of little faith’, headed by the adjective katan, can never be ktan emun-ot, with plurality marked only on the noun.

  6. There are also some adjectives with -i that are not derived from a noun, e.g. ʁiʃm-i ‘official’ and loans like patét-i ‘pathetic’. Interestingly, it may be claimed these are also perceived as complex, as evidenced by cases of back-formation like patet ‘pathetic person’.

  7. Interestingly, tat muda ‘sub-conscious’ is derived from an adjectival templatic passive muda ‘conscious (adj.)’, but the item is a noun, not an adjective. Here is the place to mention a couple of marginal exceptions pointed out by a reviewer, e.g. tat-adom ‘sub-red’ and tat-zaiʁ ‘sub-miniscule’.

  8. A reviewer asks why, given (16) and the analogy between tat nouns and the Nhead+Nmodifier construction, the latter type of item is never subject to adjectivization, e.g. pkid mas-i ‘of a tax clerk’ (though there are rare examples such as bejt sifr-i ‘of school’, derived from bejt séfer ‘school’). This interesting question belongs to a larger study on the formation of adjectives in -i.

    A related issue is the following. Templatic adjectives can be derived from nouns, e.g. mélaχ ‘salt’ => malúaχ ‘salted’. In principle, the adjectival head in (16) could have been expressed by a template, rather than -i. But in that case, it would apply to tat kaʁka in its entirety, including the prefixoid tat. Attachment of -i is the preferred strategy, if only on the basis of the transparency of the outcome. Adjectives like tat-malúaχ are not expected to exist.

  9. Siloni (2001) argues that the position of ha is determined “in the phonology”. A similar stance is taken by Borer (2013).

  10. Another possible motivation for this movement is prosodic. tat is never stressed; if it did not move with its complement, it would be stranded in the phrase-final position in which, like a modifier N (e.g. pkìd más ‘tax clerk’), it would have to carry main stress. I will not have more to say about this movement.

  11. The structure with tat seleting a null head cannot serve as the basis for adjectival formation; this would derive the contentless tat-i.

  12. There might be good phonological reason for gender to be absent from tat: the default gender markers in MH constructs are /-at/ and /-ot/. Their suffixation would yield ?tat-at and ?tat-ot, which would be subject to OCP considerations due to the segmental similarity with the base. Similarly, the particle du ‘bi’ in du kʁav ‘duel’, flatly rejects the plural suffix, probably because it is vowel-final: *du-ej kʁav (cf. χad ‘mono’ in χad kéʁen ‘unicorn’, χad-ej kéʁen ‘unicorns’).

  13. If features can be inherent on pro, one expects also cases of singular tat tzuna which do not trigger feminine agreement, e.g. tat tzunafsg mezaazéamsg ‘shocking under-nutrition’. I have indeed found such cases.

References

  • Bauer, L., Rochelle, L., & Plag, I. (2013). The Oxford reference guide to English morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (1999). Deconstructing the construct. In K. Johnson & I. Roberts (Eds.), Beyond principles and parameters: essays in memory of Osvaldo Jaeggli (pp. 43–89). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (2008). Compounds: the view from Hebrew. In R. Lieber & P. Stekauer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compounds, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (2013). The syntactic domain of content. In M. Becker, J. Grinstead, J. Rothmann, & B. D. Schwartz (Eds.), Generative linguistics and acquisition: studies in honor of Nina M. Hyams (pp. 205–248). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1970). Remarks on nominalization. In R. Jacobs & P. Rosenbaum (Eds.), Reading in English transformational grammar (pp. 184–221). Waltham: Ginn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danon, G. (2008). Definiteness spreading in the Hebrew construct state. Lingua, 118(7), 872–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D. (2010). Localism versus globalism in morphology and phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, N. (2014). Where it’s [at]: a phonological correlate to the Phase Impenetrability Condition in the Construct State of Modern Hebrew. Lingua, 150, 315–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, N. (2018). Exponence, allomorphy and haplology in the number and State morphology of Modern Hebrew. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 48. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gadish, R. (2016). (Sub-committee, ambiguous: on prefixes and suffixes). Post on the official Hebrew Academy web-page, accessed 19/06/2020.

  • Goldenberg, G. (1995). Attribution in Semitic languages. Langues Orientales Anciennes: Philologie et Linguistique, 7–20.

  • Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20 (pp. 111–176). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahana, C. (1998). . PhD dissertation, Hebrew University.

  • Kramer, R. (2015). The morphosyntax of gender. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenstamm, J. (2014). Derivational affixes as roots: phasal spellout meets English stress shift. In A. Alexiadou, H. Borer, & F. Schafer (Eds.), The syntax of roots and the roots of syntax (pp. 230–258). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, H. (2019). Bracketing paradoxes in morphology. In R. Lieber, S. Arndt-Lappe, A. Fàbergas, C. Gagné, & F. Masini (Eds.), The Oxford encyclopedia of morphology, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritter, E. (1988). A head-movement approach to construct state noun phrases. Linguistics, 26(6), 909–929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. (2002). (studies in Hebrew morphology). Tel Aviv: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shlonsky, Ur. (2004). The form of semitic nominals. Lingua, 114(12), 1465–1526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siloni, T. (2001). Construct states at the PF interface. Linguistic Variation Yearbook, 1, 229–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

I thank Bar Avineri for reading a draft of this paper and commenting on it, and for encouraging me to write it.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Noam Faust.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Faust, N. Modern Hebrew prefixoids: description and morpho-syntactic analysis. Morphology 32, 25–44 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-021-09385-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-021-09385-7

Keywords

Navigation