Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of linguistic medium on metaphor directionality: written standard Arabic versus oral colloquial Arabic

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Numerous scholars have demonstrated the existence of pervasive directionality in verbal metaphors, for example when comparing the two terms ‘conscience’ and ‘compass’, the preferred direction would be ‘Conscience is a compass’, while saying ‘A compass in conscience’ would be anomalous Most of the theories in this field claim that the directionality of metaphors stems from the conceptual level (Lakoff and Johnson, in Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press, 1980; in philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. HarperCollins, 1999), while recently, a few researchers have pointed to the critical influence of different grammatical structures on directionality (Shen & Porat, in Handbook of categorization in cognitive science, chapter 47. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-101107-2.00047-6, 2017; Gil and Shen, Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 10, 2275). Such findings showing the significant impact of linguistic factors on metaphor directionality lead to the hypothesis that different linguistic modalities can affect metaphor directionality. In this study, we examined for the first time the effect of the linguistic medium on directionality, mainly focusing on modality (written vs. oral). The language chosen for the study was Arabic, which, because of its diglossic state, has one of the biggest differences between the two modalities in the daily life of its speakers. Two Arabic-speaking groups were asked to produce asymmetric similes using 32 metaphoric pairs: one group performed the task in written form using Modern Standard Arabic, and the other in oral form using Colloquial Palestinian Arabic, and then computing the percentage of the similes emitted in the preferred direction (directionality). The results showed a significant difference in directionality percentages between the two mediums favoring the written MSA, in accordance with our hypothesis. The findings are discussed with regard to metaphor directionality theory, as well as the effect of the linguistic medium.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The authors confirm that the data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its supplementary materials.

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing,16, 423–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayari, S. (1996). Diglossia and illiteracy in the Arab world. Language Culture and Curriculum,9, 243–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benítez-Burraco, A., Cahuana, C., Chen, S., Gil, D., Progovac, L., Reifegerste, J., & Tatiana Tatarinova. (2022). Cognitive and genetic correlates of a single macro-parameter of crosslinguistic variation. In The evolution of language, proceedings of the 14th international conference (EVOLANG14).

  • Biber, D. (2011). Speech and writing: Linguistic styles enabled by the technology of literacy. In G. Andersen & K. Aijmer (Eds.), The pragmatics of Society (pp. 137–152). de Gruyter Mouton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cerbin, W. (1985). Young children’s comprehension of metaphoric language. Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto.

  • Clark, H. H. (2004). Pragmatics of language performance. In R. L. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (pp. 365–382). Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor, K. (1983). Literal and figurative comparisons: Developmental patterns in preference for direction. Paper presented at the Biennial meeting of the SRCD, Detroit.

  • Connor, K., & Martin, A. J. (1982). Children’s recognition of asymmetry in metaphor. Paper presented at the Meeting of the South-Eastern Conference on Human Development, Baltimore.

  • Connor, K., & Kogan, N. (1980). Topic-vehicle relations in metaphor: The issue of asymmetry. In R. P. Honeck & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cognition and figurative language (pp. 283–310). Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deane, P. D. (1992). Grammar in mind and brain: Explorations in cognitive syntax. Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2000). Bilingual is as bilingual does: Metalinguistic abilities of arabic-speaking children. Applied Psychology,21(4), 451–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, & Bretz, Hothorn, T. and Westfall, P. (2010). Multiple comparisons using R. CRC Press.

  • Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,6, 78–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Bowdle, B. (2008). Metaphor as structure-mapping. In The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought, edited by Raymond W. Gibbs Jr., 109–28. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil, D. (2019). “Multilingualism”. In S.A. Adelaar and A. Schapper (Eds) The Malayo-Polynesian languages of Southeast Asia. Oxford University Press.

  • Gil, D., & Shen, Y. (2019). How grammar introduces asymmetry into cognitive structures: Compositional semantics, metaphors, and schematological hybrids. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 10, 2275. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02275.

  • Gil, D. (2021). Tense-aspect-mood marking, language-family size and the evolution of predication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B,376, 20200194. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0194)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gil, D., & Shen, Y. (2021). Metaphors: The evolutionary journey from bidirectionality to unidirectionality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glucksberg, S., & Keysar, B. (1990). Understanding metaphorical comparisons: Beyond similarity. Psychological Review,97(1), 3–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glucksberg, S., McGlone, M. S., & Manfredi, D. (1997). Property attribution in metaphor comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language,36(1), 50–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, T. J., & Pregibon, D. (1992). In S. Chambers, & T. J. Hastie (Eds.), Generalized linear models. Chapter 6 of statistical models. Wadsworth & Brooks/Cole.

  • Holes, C. (2004). Modern Arabic: structures, functions, and varieties. Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal,50(3), 346–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ibrahim, R. (2009). The cognitive basis of diglossia in Arabic: Evidence from a repetition priming study within and between languages. Psychology Research and Behavior Management,2, 93–105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, J. (1996). Multiple Comparisons: Theory and Methods (1st ed.). Chapman and Hall/CRC. https://doi.org/10.1201/b15074

  • Kadan, J. (2019). Conceptualization differences between writing and speaking: The case of diglossia in Arabic Bilinguals in a Visual Hybrid Description Task, MA Thesis, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv.

  • Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological perspective. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Khamis-Dakwar, R., Froud, K., & Gordon, P. (2012). Acquiring diglossia: Mutual influences of formal and colloquial Arabic on children’s grammaticality judgments. Journal of Child Language,39, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kogan, N., & Chadrow, M. (2012). and Harbour, H. (1989). Developmental trends in metaphoric asymmetry. Metaphor Symbolic Activity, 4 (2):71–91

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maamouri, M. (1998). Language education and human development: Arabic diglossia and its impact on the quality of education in the Arab region. In Paper presented at the Mediterranean development forum. Marrakech, Morocco.

  • Mashal, N., Shen, Y., Jospe, K., & Gil, D. (2014). Language effects on the conceptualization of hybrids. Language and Cognition,6, 217–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McWhorter, J. (2007). Language interrupted: Signs of Non-native acquisition in standard Language Grammars. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, T. F. (1986). What is educated spoken arabic? International Journal of the Sociology of Language,61, 7–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. (1994). The world on paper. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A. (1979). In A. Ortony (Ed.), “Metaphor: A multidimensional problem, “ in Metaphor and Thought (pp. 1–16). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., Richard, J. V., Mark, A. F., & Lawrence, E. J. (1985). Salience, similes, and the asymmetry of similarity. Journal of Memory and Kanguage,24(5), 569–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porat, R., & Shen, Y. (2017). The journey from bidirectionality to unidirectionality. Poetics Today,38, 123–140. https://doi.org/10.1215/03335372-3716252)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2012). Spelling morphology: The psycholinguistics of hebrew spelling (Chap. 1). Tel-Aviv University. IL: Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2012.

  • Ravid, D., & Chen-Djemal, Y. (2015). Spoken and written narration in Hebrew. Written Language & Literacy,18(1), 56–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. H. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology,4(3), 328–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90017-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenhouse, Y. (1997). Morphological and syntactical acquisition of spoken arabic as native language: Preliminary findings [in Hebrew]. Hilkat Lashon,27, 76–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics,24, 431–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of words and pseudowords in a diglossic context. Applied Psycholinguistics,25, 495–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, Y., & Gil, D. (2010). The perception of visual hybrids: the role of language. Paper presented at Cognitive Poetics and Rhetoric 1.0. University of Lodz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shen, Y., and Gil, D. (2017). How language influences the way we categorize hybrids. Handbook of categorization in cognitive science, Chap. 47. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-101107-2.00047-6.

  • Shen, Y., & Porat, R. (2017). Metaphorical directionality: the role of language, in metaphor: embodied cognition and discourse, edited by Beate Hampe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. (1996). From ‘thought and language’ to ‘thinking for speaking.’ In J. Gumperz & S. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I., Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 157–192). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, P. B. (2006). Is Spanish really so easy? Is Arabic really so hard? Perceived difficulty in learning Arabic as a second language. In K. M. Wahba, A. T. Zeinab, & L. England (Eds.), Handbook for Arabic language teaching professionals in the 21st century (pp. 35–63). London, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

  • Suleiman, S. M. (1986). Jordanian arabic between diglossia and bilingualism: Linguistic analysis. John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review,84(4), 327–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P., & Gentner, D. (2000). Evidence for role-neutral initial processing of metaphors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition,26(2), 529–541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P., & Gentner, D. (2011). Structure-mapping in metaphor comprehension. Cognitive Science,35.8, 1456–1488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank my doctoral advisors Professor David Gil of the Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution at Max Planck Institute for The Science and Human History, and Professor Yeshayahu Shen in the Program of the Cognitive Studies of Language Use and the Department of Literature at Tel-Aviv University. To me, they are much more than advisors; they are true teachers who opened my eyes and enlightened me throughout my doctoral studies. Furthermore, they patiently and consistently guided me through all the hard problems I faced from beginning to end, in this specific study and many others.

Funding

The author received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JK designed the study, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jad Kiadan.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author certifies that he has no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest or non-financial interest in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Appendix

Appendix

Full list of the experiment items with the averages of canonical responses for each item.

figure a

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kiadan, J. The effect of linguistic medium on metaphor directionality: written standard Arabic versus oral colloquial Arabic. J Cult Cogn Sci 8, 65–78 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-023-00135-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-023-00135-1

Keywords

Navigation