02.09.2015 Views

DIAGNOSTIC TEST OF WORD READING PROCESSES

diagnostic test of word reading processes - GL Assessment

diagnostic test of word reading processes - GL Assessment

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>DIAGNOSTIC</strong><br />

<strong>TEST</strong> <strong>OF</strong><br />

<strong>WORD</strong><br />

<strong>READING</strong><br />

<strong>PROCESSES</strong><br />

Evaluation Teacher’s Guide Pack<br />

Forum for Research in Literacy and Language, Institute of Education


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

Welcome to the Diagnostic Test of<br />

Word Reading Processes<br />

<strong>DIAGNOSTIC</strong><br />

<strong>TEST</strong> <strong>OF</strong><br />

<strong>WORD</strong><br />

<strong>READING</strong><br />

<strong>PROCESSES</strong><br />

Teacher’s Guide<br />

Forum for Research in Literacy and Language, Institute of Education<br />

Contents<br />

Introduction to the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes 03<br />

Example reading cards 04<br />

An essential tutorial 06<br />

Examples 13<br />

Order form 16<br />

2


Evaluation Pack<br />

Introduction to the<br />

Diagnostic Test of Word Reading<br />

Processes<br />

The Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes is a test of word and non-word reading for children<br />

aged 6 to 12.<br />

The test comprises 90 items divided as follows:<br />

• 30 exception words which provide a measure of lexical-semantic processes;<br />

• 30 non-words which provide a measure of phonological recoding processes;<br />

• 30 regular words which can be read by either process.<br />

It is the first test to:<br />

• match words in the regular word and non-word sets by compiling non-words from segments of<br />

regular words;<br />

• match the regular and exception word sets according to their frequency of occurrence in books<br />

read by school children in the UK;<br />

• provide normative data obtained from a large and representative sample of children from<br />

Reception/P1 to Y7/S1 in schools in England.<br />

Quick and easy to administer, score and interpret, the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

makes available to teachers a diagnostic tool that has long been used in research studies, and that<br />

enables precise specification of the areas of difficulty experienced by individual children in developing<br />

fluent and automatic word reading.<br />

Developed by the Forum for Research in Literacy and Language and used as a research tool over<br />

several years, the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes standardisation was carried out in<br />

2009 and 2011 and the test yields an overall standard age score, age equivalent scores and an<br />

individual stanine score for each of its three component parts.<br />

3


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

Sample non-words<br />

Example<br />

reading<br />

cards<br />

Practice items<br />

vip tek<br />

yol hap cug vus rit<br />

kem jid wex bof naz<br />

Please note: These are not actual words from the test, but are close approximations to the test content.<br />

NW1<br />

4


Evaluation Pack<br />

NW2<br />

Sample non-words II<br />

froll mong merfar hister nostchen<br />

hunril pringy kitcess bungentine turpalow<br />

Sample exception words<br />

do bear what people wolf<br />

eye country wealth rheumatism beautiful<br />

Sample regular words<br />

frog hill monster farmer kitchen<br />

nostril hungry princess bungalow turpentine<br />

Please note: These are not actual words from the test, but are close approximations to the test content.<br />

5


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

An essential tutorial<br />

Introduction to the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

What is involved in learning to read?<br />

There are two essential dimensions to learning to read: children must learn to recognise<br />

and understand written words, and they must learn to recover and interpret the literal and<br />

inferential meanings of texts. These two dimensions are encapsulated in the Simple View<br />

of Reading (Hoover and Gough, 1990). 3<br />

Figure 1.1: The Simple View of Reading<br />

Language<br />

Comprehension<br />

Processes<br />

Good<br />

Word<br />

Word<br />

Recognition<br />

Poor<br />

Good<br />

Recognition<br />

Processes<br />

Processes<br />

Poor<br />

Language<br />

Comprehension<br />

Processes<br />

Each dimension within this simple framework is far from ‘simple’, and involves the<br />

development and use of complex cognitive processes – processes operating within the<br />

mind and brain.<br />

6<br />

3<br />

Useful sources of information about the Simple View of Reading include: Adlof et al. (2006); Hoover and Gough (1990); Kendeou et al.<br />

(2009); Kirby and Savage (2008); Stuart et al. (2008).


Evaluation Pack<br />

What does the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes assess?<br />

The test focuses on the cognitive processes that underlie the recognition and<br />

understanding of written words: the ‘word recognition’ dimension of the Simple View<br />

of Reading.<br />

It does this by assessing pupils’ performance on three types of word:<br />

• Regular words, for example ‘meet’, ‘shampoo’, ‘catapult’, which conform to the<br />

grapheme-phoneme correspondence (GPC) rules of the language<br />

• Exception words, for example, ‘was’, ‘yacht’, ‘meringue’, which do not conform to<br />

these GPC rules<br />

• Non-words, for example ‘brafe’, ‘meckton’, ‘shelibowt’, which are pronounceable<br />

sequences of letters that do not exist as real words in the language.<br />

What are these word reading processes?<br />

Over the past 50 years, there has been a huge amount of research devoted to<br />

understanding how people are able to recognise the individual sequences of letters that<br />

constitute each written word, and thereby gain access to the meaning and pronunciation<br />

of each word. Some areas of difference in detail remain between different theories.<br />

However, there is now a large measure of agreement that skilled readers have developed<br />

two interacting sets of processes that operate together on each written word. Therefore,<br />

pupils learning to read need to develop both sets of processes if they are to become<br />

skilled readers.<br />

In order to remain impartial with respect to the remaining differences between competing<br />

theories, the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes uses the neutral terms ‘lexicalsemantic”<br />

and ‘phonological’ to describe these two sets of interacting processes. To make<br />

best use of the test, you need to understand what each set of processes involves. A brief<br />

account is provided below.<br />

What are lexical-semantic processes?<br />

Lexical-semantic processes depend upon accessing previously stored knowledge about<br />

words.<br />

Long before they start to learn to read, children have already stored the meanings and<br />

pronunciations of thousands of words that they use in their daily lives. Where a word is<br />

already stored in a pupil’s oral vocabulary, developing lexical-semantic processes involves:<br />

• storing knowledge of the letter sequence of the written form of the word<br />

• linking the letter sequence to its meaning.<br />

Word meanings already stored in oral vocabulary are already linked to their pronunciation.<br />

This means that accessing the meaning of the word automatically triggers access to its<br />

pronunciation.<br />

7


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

However, reading soon becomes a major source of oral vocabulary development, and so<br />

pupils start to come across written words whose meanings and pronunciations are not<br />

already in their oral vocabulary. In these cases, developing lexical-semantic processes<br />

involves:<br />

• storing knowledge of the letter sequence of the written word<br />

• inferring something of the word’s meaning from the context in which it occurs<br />

• working out a pronunciation for the word from its letter sequence<br />

• linking the letter sequence to the word’s meaning and pronunciation.<br />

And, of course, unfamiliar words encountered in print may well be mispronounced: for<br />

example, ‘misled’ may become ‘mizzled’, and give rise to a new verb ‘to mizzle’ (as<br />

evidenced in the speech error ‘don’t mizzle me’).<br />

Once the letter sequence of a previously unknown written word has been stored and<br />

linked to its meaning and pronunciation, on subsequent encounters that written word<br />

will be recognised immediately on sight. Instant recognition leads to automatic and<br />

effortless recovery of the stored meaning and pronunciation. Lexical-semantic processing<br />

is extraordinarily fast and operates without any conscious awareness on the part of the<br />

skilled reader.<br />

Lexical-semantic processes are involved in reading all words, both regular and exception. 4<br />

However, they are uniquely tapped by Exception Word Reading, as Exception Word<br />

Reading is only achieved through lexical-semantic processing. Therefore, Exception Word<br />

Reading is used in this test as the measure of lexical-semantic processes.<br />

Importantly, the sequence of operations in lexical-semantic processing is from stored letter<br />

sequence to word meaning to word pronunciation. Thus, these processes allow us to<br />

distinguish between homophones like ‘sail’ and ‘sale’, even when we read them without<br />

any disambiguating context. The letter sequence ‘sail’ is linked to the meaning ‘an area of<br />

fabric used to catch the wind and propel a boat over water’, whereas the letter sequence<br />

‘sale’ is linked to the meaning ‘the exchange of goods, property or services for an agreed<br />

sum of money or credit’.<br />

8<br />

4<br />

The only requirement is that words should be familiar to the reader. That is, they must have been previously encountered in print<br />

sufficiently often for their letter sequences, meanings and pronunciations to have been stored. In skilled readers, one or two encounters<br />

with a written word are sufficient. For beginner readers, many more encounters are needed: some young beginner readers fail to<br />

recognise and understand written words that they have seen very many times previously (see, for example, Stuart et al., 2000).


Evaluation Pack<br />

Figure 1.2: Sequence of operations in lexical-semantic processing<br />

Written<br />

word<br />

SAIL<br />

Access stored<br />

letter sequence<br />

Access stored<br />

word meaning<br />

S-A-I-L<br />

‘an area of fabric<br />

used to catch the<br />

wind and propel<br />

a boat over<br />

water’<br />

Access stored<br />

word<br />

pronunciation<br />

/seɪl/<br />

S-A-L-E<br />

‘the exchange of<br />

goods, property<br />

or services for an<br />

agreed sum of<br />

money or credit’<br />

Access stored<br />

word<br />

pronunciation<br />

SALE<br />

Written<br />

word<br />

Access stored<br />

letter sequence<br />

Access stored<br />

word meaning<br />

What are phonological processes in reading?<br />

Phonological processes depend on the use of previously stored knowledge about<br />

correspondences between printed letters and letter groups (graphemes) and speech<br />

sounds (phonemes): that is, stored knowledge of grapheme-phoneme correspondence<br />

(GPC) rules. They are involved in reading regular words, whether or not these have been<br />

previously seen and stored, and non-words. Using phonological processes to read<br />

exception words would lead to a mispronunciation, for example reading ‘yacht’ to rhyme<br />

with ‘matched’. Phonological processes are uniquely tapped by Non-word Reading,<br />

because non-words do not exist as words in the language. They have not been seen<br />

before, and so their letter sequence cannot have been stored. They can only be read<br />

accurately by phonological processing. Therefore, Non-word Reading is used in the<br />

Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes as the measure of phonological processing.<br />

Development of phonological processes for word reading involves:<br />

• developing an understanding of the alphabetic principle, that phonemes in spoken<br />

words are represented by graphemes in written words<br />

• developing explicit awareness of the sequence of phonemes that constitute each<br />

spoken word<br />

• learning the correspondences between graphemes and phonemes<br />

9


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

• learning to recognise graphemes in written words<br />

• learning to blend phonemes into whole-word pronunciations.<br />

Importantly, the sequence of operations in phonological processing is from letter<br />

sequences to grapheme-phoneme translation and phoneme blending, to word<br />

pronunciation and thence to word meaning.<br />

Figure 1.3: Sequence of operations in phonological processing<br />

Written<br />

word<br />

Identify<br />

graphemes<br />

SHAMPOO<br />

sh-a-m-p-oo<br />

Translate<br />

graphemes to<br />

phonemes<br />

sh ➔ /ʃ/<br />

a ➔ /a/<br />

m ➔ /m/<br />

p ➔ /p/<br />

oo ➔ /u/<br />

/ʃ-a-m-p-u/<br />

/ʃampu/<br />

Blend phonemes<br />

Access stored<br />

word<br />

pronunciation<br />

/ʃampu/<br />

Access stored<br />

word meaning<br />

‘soap to wash<br />

the hair’<br />

Developing and learning the necessary components for phonological processes to<br />

operate fluently can be a difficult task for some pupils. But phonological processes offer<br />

rich rewards for the reader. Learning a relatively small 5 set of GPC rules allows the pupil<br />

to work out pronunciations for the increasing numbers of unfamiliar written words they<br />

encounter. The meanings of many of these words may already be stored in the pupil’s oral<br />

vocabulary. Thus, working out the pronunciation allows the pupil to access the meaning<br />

of the word. As with lexical-semantic processes, something of the word’s meaning can be<br />

inferred from the context in which it is encountered, and stored for future use.<br />

In skilled readers, phonological processes operate automatically and fluently, without the<br />

conscious awareness of the reader.<br />

10<br />

5<br />

See, for example, Vousden et al., 2010.


Evaluation Pack<br />

How does the test assess lexical-semantic processes?<br />

The test contains sets of matched regular words and exception words. Pupils whose<br />

reading relies on the use of lexical-semantic processes will read regular and exception<br />

words to a broadly equivalent level, because both can be accurately pronounced by<br />

lexical-semantic processing. Pupils who also have well-developed phonological processes<br />

will tend to show an advantage for regular over exception words. This is because regular<br />

words can be pronounced accurately by both sets of processes, and so do not need to<br />

be already known and stored by the reader.<br />

As explained above, Exception Word Reading is the purest measure of lexical-semantic<br />

processes. Therefore, in the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes, stanine scores 6<br />

for Exception Word Reading are used as the measure of lexical-semantic processes.<br />

How does the test assess phonological processes?<br />

The test also contains a set of non-words: letter sequences that could be words in the<br />

language, but that do not link to any meaning. The non-words in this test have been<br />

made by recombining letter sequences from the words in the regular word set: thus, all<br />

their component letter sequences are as familiar or unfamiliar to the pupil as the letter<br />

sequences in the regular words. Pupils whose reading relies predominantly on the use of<br />

phonological processes will read regular words and non-words to a broadly equivalent<br />

level, because both can be pronounced accurately by phonological processing. Their<br />

reading of exception words, which can only be pronounced accurately by lexical-semantic<br />

processes, will be relatively poorer.<br />

As explained above, Non-word Reading is the purest measure of phonological processes.<br />

Therefore, in the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes, stanine scores for Non-word<br />

Reading are used as the measure of phonological processes.<br />

Why should I use the test?<br />

The Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes is the only standardised test with British<br />

English norms currently available that allows you to uncover the nature of the difficulties<br />

an individual pupil is experiencing in developing fluent automatic word reading processes.<br />

The pupil profile provided by administering this test can usefully inform your ability to<br />

provide teaching that is appropriate to the needs of the individual pupil, and that will help<br />

the pupil to overcome his or her difficulties.<br />

6<br />

See page 22 of the Teacher’s Guide for a definition of stanine scores.<br />

11


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

When should I use the test?<br />

You can use the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes to assess the word reading<br />

skills of all the pupils in your class, or you might prefer to use it selectively with pupils<br />

whose reading development is causing you concern.<br />

The Exception Word Reading/Non-word Reading comparisons reveal three different kinds<br />

of profile of reading difficulties:<br />

• a Lexical-Semantic profile, indicating relative difficulty in developing lexical-semantic<br />

processes<br />

• a Phonological profile, indicating a relative difficulty in developing phonological<br />

processes<br />

• a Mixed profile, indicating difficulty in developing both sets of processes.<br />

Procedures for obtaining and interpreting these profiles are described in Chapter 4.<br />

Suggestions for what to do once you have obtained and interpreted a profile for an<br />

individual pupil are given in Chapter 6.<br />

It is important to remember that the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes is<br />

focused on the word reading dimension of the Simple View of Reading. It cannot<br />

(and is not intended to) identify pupils who are experiencing difficulties with reading<br />

comprehension, except in so far as those difficulties may be caused by poor or inefficient<br />

word reading skills. Pupils with age appropriate word reading skills who struggle to<br />

understand written texts appropriate to their age and interests should be given a prose<br />

reading test such as the York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension (YARC, Snowling<br />

et al., 2009).<br />

12


Evaluation Pack<br />

Examples using words supplied in the<br />

Evaluation Pack<br />

Example 1<br />

Exception Regular Non-word<br />

do<br />

√<br />

frog<br />

√<br />

froll<br />

√<br />

bear<br />

√<br />

hill<br />

√<br />

mong<br />

√<br />

what<br />

√<br />

monster x merfar x<br />

people<br />

√<br />

farmer<br />

√<br />

hister x<br />

wolf x kitchen<br />

√<br />

nostchen x<br />

eye<br />

√<br />

nostril x hunril x<br />

country x hungry<br />

√<br />

pringry x<br />

wealth x princess x kitcess x<br />

rheumatism x bungalow x bungentine x<br />

beautiful x turpentine x turpalow x<br />

Exception word<br />

Regular word<br />

Non-word<br />

= 5 correct<br />

= 5 correct<br />

= 2 correct<br />

From these raw scores it looks as though this child reads exception words and regular words equally<br />

well, and that he/she reads words better than non-words. However, raw scores are misleading and<br />

you cannot draw any conclusions from them. You need to convert raw scores to stanine scores using<br />

the normative data tables.<br />

Using stanine scores, the test is designed to reveal three patterns (‘profiles’) of word reading. This can<br />

be done by simply comparing the child’s stanine score for exception words with their stanine score for<br />

non-words.<br />

Let’s imagine that the child in the example above achieved a stanine score of 3 for exception word<br />

reading, and 1 for non-word reading.<br />

As you can see, these stanine scores give the child a<br />

phonological profile. The child relies mostly in reading on<br />

lexical-semantic processes, which can read all words, regular<br />

as well as exception, as long as they are already familiar<br />

to the child. Unfamiliar words cannot be decoded using<br />

phonological recoding processes. Intervention for this child<br />

should concentrate on developing his/her phonological<br />

recoding processes.<br />

13


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

Example 2<br />

Exception Regular Non-word<br />

do<br />

√<br />

frog<br />

√<br />

froll<br />

√<br />

bear<br />

x<br />

hill<br />

√<br />

mong<br />

√<br />

what<br />

√<br />

monster<br />

√<br />

merfar<br />

√<br />

people<br />

√<br />

farmer<br />

√<br />

hister<br />

√<br />

wolf x kitchen<br />

√<br />

nostchen<br />

√<br />

eye x nostril<br />

√<br />

hunril<br />

√<br />

country x hungry x pringry x<br />

wealth x princess<br />

√<br />

kitcess x<br />

rheumatism x bungalow x bungentine x<br />

beautiful x turpentine x turpalow x<br />

Exception word<br />

Regular word<br />

Non-word<br />

= 3 correct<br />

= 7 correct<br />

= 6 correct<br />

From these raw scores it looks as though this child reads regular words and non-words equally well,<br />

but finds exception words difficult.<br />

However, raw scores are misleading and you cannot draw any conclusions from them.<br />

You need to convert raw scores to stanine scores using the normative data tables.<br />

Using stanine scores, the test is designed to reveal three patterns (‘profiles’) of word reading.<br />

This can be done by simply comparing the child’s stanine score for exception words with their stanine<br />

score for non-words.<br />

Let’s imagine that the child in the example above achieved a stanine score of 2 for exception word<br />

reading, and 5 for non-word reading.<br />

As you can see, these stanine scores give the child a<br />

lexical-semantic profile. He/she relies mostly in reading on<br />

phonological recoding processes, which can read nonwords<br />

and regular words, whether or not the latter are already<br />

familiar to the child. However, he/she has difficulty with<br />

exception words, as these can only be read accurately by<br />

lexical-semantic processes. Intervention for this child should<br />

concentrate on developing his/her lexical-semantic processes.<br />

14


Evaluation Pack<br />

Example 3<br />

Exception Regular Non-word<br />

do<br />

√<br />

frog<br />

√<br />

froll<br />

√<br />

bear x hill<br />

√<br />

mong<br />

√<br />

what<br />

√<br />

monster x merfar x<br />

people<br />

√<br />

farmer x hister x<br />

wolf x kitchen x nostchen x<br />

eye x nostril x hunril x<br />

country x hungry x pringry x<br />

wealth x princess x kitcess x<br />

rheumatism x bungalow x bungentine x<br />

beautiful x turpentine x turpalow x<br />

Exception word<br />

Regular word<br />

Non-word<br />

= 3 correct<br />

= 2 correct<br />

= 2 correct<br />

From these raw scores it looks as though this child has equal difficulty reading all three types of word.<br />

However, raw scores are misleading and you cannot draw any conclusions from them.<br />

You need to convert raw scores to stanine scores using the normative data tables.<br />

Using stanine scores, the test is designed to reveal three patterns (‘profiles’) of word reading.<br />

This can be done by simply comparing the child’s stanine score for exception words with their stanine<br />

score for non-words.<br />

Let’s imagine that the child in the example above achieved a stanine score of 2 for exception word<br />

reading, and 1 for non-word reading.<br />

As you can see, these stanine scores give the child a mixed<br />

profile. He/she has not developed either set of word reading<br />

processes to an age-appropriate level, and has no relative<br />

strength or weakness in one or other set. Intervention for this<br />

child should concentrate on developing both phonological<br />

recoding and lexical-semantic processes.<br />

15


Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

Using the Diagnostic<br />

Test of Word Reading<br />

Processes and<br />

Phonological<br />

Assessment Battery<br />

together<br />

Secure your copy of<br />

the brand new Diagnostic Test of<br />

Word Reading Processes<br />

4 easy ways to order<br />

1) Photocopy and fax this order form to 0845 601 5358<br />

2) Post it to: GL Assessment, FREEPOST LON16517, Swindon SN2 8BR<br />

3) Call 0845 602 1937<br />

4) Visit www.gl-assessment.co.uk and enter DTWRP or PhAB into the Search<br />

box (no credit card required – we invoice your school)<br />

+<br />

For advice on the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes or any of our other<br />

assessments, or to arrange a visit from your local Area Consultant to discuss your<br />

reading assessment needs in more detail, please call 0845 602 1937 or send an email<br />

to info@gl-assessment.co.uk. Alternatively, find your local Area Consultant online at<br />

www.gl-assessment.co.uk/findyourrep.<br />

Mr/Mrs/Miss/Ms<br />

Job Title<br />

Address<br />

Post Code<br />

GL Assessment’s Phonological<br />

Assessment Battery (PhAB) is an<br />

ideal follow-on assessment for<br />

some children tested with the<br />

Diagnostic Test of Word Reading<br />

Processes. The latter is designed<br />

to benchmark and test pupils’<br />

developing skills in word reading<br />

and can be used to track progress<br />

as part of a school’s assessment<br />

regime. If persistent problems are<br />

detected then the specialist<br />

teacher/SENCO could administer<br />

PhAB, a well-established test that<br />

looks at six different areas of<br />

phonological processing.<br />

Tel<br />

Email<br />

Official Order No.<br />

Paying Authority/School<br />

Please invoice me Cheque enclosed Amount: £ ___________<br />

(payable to GL Assessment)<br />

Title Code Price Qty Total<br />

Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes<br />

Complete Set 978 07087 20721/GLA449 £100.00*<br />

Record Forms (Pack of 50) 978 07087 20738/GLA449 £35.00*<br />

Phonological Assessment Battery (PhAB)<br />

Complete Set 978 07087 07081/GLA449 £119.99*<br />

Record Forms (Pack of 10) 978 07087 07098/GLA449 £17.99*<br />

Fax<br />

Please contact us for more<br />

information:<br />

T: 0845 602 1937<br />

E: info@gl-assessment.co.uk<br />

*+VAT<br />

**UK postage and packing: Add £4.15 if enclosing payment<br />

with your order (If you are not enclosing payment, you will be<br />

charged the full cost of postage based on weight and<br />

delivery date required.)<br />

Sub Total<br />

P&P**<br />

+VAT @ 20%<br />

TOTAL<br />

GLA475 GLA449

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!